EugeneA.Weinsteinn/an/an/an/a
Weights Assigned by Children to Criteria of
Prestige1
This study is concerned with children’s conceptions of a
general stratification system based upon the prestige associated with
occupational positions. Research on the patterns of adult evaluation has
revealed considerable consensus as to the relative prestige of many
occupations. It may be assumed that consensus in evaluation is the
product of a common learning process. However, it is insufficient to point
only to a common learning process as an explanatory factor. The content of
this process must be examined if its relationship to evaluation is to be
understood. By internalizing this content and learning to respond to the
conception of a general hierarchy, the individual is learning a role: that
of an ascriber of prestige. The purpose of this study is to investigate one
aspect of the development of that role.
Prestige, as defined in this study, is multi-dimensional. Following
Hatt’s usage, some combination of all the rewards and prerequisites of a
position constitutes its invidious value and hence its prestige. Thus these
rewards and prerequisites serve as criteria of prestige and are of special
interest here. From a survey of the literature on stratification, seven
such criteria have been isolated. They are the income, education, working
conditions, fame, community service, authority, and scarcity of personnel
associated with a given occupation.
Two functions are performed by the ascriber in the evaluation of
occupational prestige:
1. The ascriber must estimate the amount or
increment of each of the criteria associated with a given
occupation.
2. The ascriber must weight each criterion in accordance with its
importance in his own value system.
Thus variation in prestige ratings can come about in two ways.
First, there may be differences among ascribers as to the increment of any
criterion associated with an occupation. For example, two ascribers could
have quite different conceptions as to the amount of authority invested in
a shop foreman. Second, the weights assigned to each criterion might not be
the same for every group of ascribers. Income might have greater invidious
value for persons of lower socio-economic status than for those of higher
status. Therefore, within this conceptual framework, a general
prestige evaluation would consist of the weighted rather than simple sum
of the rankings on each of seven criteria.
An attempt is made to determine the relative weights children assign to
prestige criteria. Of the matrix of factors related to these weights, two
have been selected for analysis in this study. (a) By systematically
varying age, it is hoped that developmental patterns in the importance
children accord specific criteria may be discovered. (b) … [Various]
studies … have indicated that one’s position in the stratification
system has important consequences for the way that system is perceived.
Similar studies have revealed striking differences between the value
systems of social classes. By varying status level, it may be possible to
determine the extent to which these differentials at the adult level are
reflected in the responses of the children.
METHOD
Sample. The elementary school population of the Chicago Public
School System constituted the universe from which the sample was drawn.
Status level was systematically varied by stratifying schools, selecting
schools most representative of the characteristics defining each stratum,
and then randomly sampling within the selected schools. The status level of
each school was defined in terms of the ecological characteristics of the
area from which it draws its students. Using census tract data on income
and education, an index was devised to measure the status level of each
area.2 The schools were then arrayed on the basis of this
index, the range computed, and divided in three. The schools were selected
as representative of the Upper, Upper-Middle, Lower-Middle and Lower status
levels, respectively.
Six white male students were drawn from each of the fourth, sixth, and
eighth grades within the selected schools. These grades correspond roughly
to ages nine, eleven, and thirteen. White males were selected to avoid the
effect of uncontrolled variation due to race and sex. Thus six students
were selected from each of three grades at four schools making a total of
seventy-two cases in all.
Interview. An interview schedule was designed to measure the
relative weight a respondent assigns to each of the seven criteria. On each
item, the respondent must rate the relative importance of two different
prestige criteria. For example, "Which do you think is more important, a
job which pays more or a job which takes more education?" Each criterion
was compared with every other criterion once, making a total of twenty-one
such paired comparisons. It was possible for any criterion to be rated
higher than other criteria from zero to six times. By defining the relative
weight of any criterion as the frequency of preference, a seven point scale
was derived.
Method of Analysis. The results of the paired comparisons test
were incorporated into analysis of variance designs; one for each
prestige criterion. A check was made to insure that the techniques used in
application met the assumptions of this method. The purpose of these
designs was to test whether the variables of grade and status level were
related to the importance assigned prestige criteria. A sample analysis for
the criterion of income appears as Table 1.
RESULTS
The results of the seven analyses of variance appear as Table 2.
Whenever the
TABLE 1
DISTRIBUTION OF WEIGHTS ASSIGNED TO INCOME BY GRADE AND STATUS
magnitude of the F ratio indicated a significant relationship between one of the
variables (status, grade, or the interaction between the two) and the weights
assigned to a particular criterion, the direction of that relationship was explored by
means of t-tests. Below are the results of these further analyses.
Income. Significant differences were found between the status groups in the
importance they accorded income. As status level increased, the weights assigned
to income decreased. The smallest difference, that between the two middle groups,
was significant, indicating significance for the entire progression.…
These patterns at the adult level, operating through the mechanisms of
socialization, may be producing similar patterns of evaluation in children.
Table 2 shows the other variables to be non-significant.
Education. Because of the necessity of education for mobility
and/or status maintenance in the Upper and Upper-Middle groups, it might be
expected that children from these groups would assign higher weight to this
criterion. On the other hand, economic pressures militate against the
availability of education as a mobility channel for members of the Lower
status group. It would be less likely that their children would accord
great importance to this factor. Education is desirable for but not
necessary to status maintenance in the Lower-Middle group. It might be
expected that children in this group would fall between the Lower and two
Upper groups. As indicated by t-tests, this was the case.
Working Conditions. As in the case of most other criteria, the
important contrast in the weights assigned to working conditions was
between the two Upper and
two Lower groups. The difference between the combined means was
significant. Factors such as dress, cleanliness of work, control over
working time, may be of secondary importance in the probable future
occupational roles of children in the Lower groups. This may be reflected
in their low frequency of preference for this criterion. Conversely,
working conditions are part of the definition of the occupational
sub-culture in the Upper and Upper-Middle groups. It might be expected that
their children would place higher value on the criterion.
Fame. Table 2 shows both status and grade to be related to the
weights assigned to fame. There is a clearly defined progression in the
importance assigned to fame at each grade level, ratings being highest in
the fourth grade, lowest in the eighth. One way of interpreting this
pattern would hold that fame’s connotations of adventure and individual
glory would have greater appeal for the egocentricity of younger children;
as they grow older and there is a reduction in egocentric perspective,
this appeal diminishes. The distribution of weights by status level was
opposite to that found for working conditions, fame having significantly
more appeal for Lower and Lower-Middle status children. The type of public
adulation connoted by fame may not be given as much emphasis as recognition
for professional achievement by those in the professions and similar
occupations. On the other hand, becoming famous, with its implications for
economic reward and personal gratification
TABLE 2
RESULTS OF ANALYSES OF VARIANCE OF WEIGHTS ASSIGNED TO PRESTIGE CRITERIA
may be equated with success by those in Lower status groups and
hence evaluated more highly.
Community Service. The results for this criterion were all
nonsignificant, scores being uniformly high for all groups. It is likely
that helping to serve the community had considerable appeal on a moral
basis. "Helping" is good, and as such it should be regarded as
important.
Authority. Both grade and status level were related to the
weights assigned to authority. Authority held significantly more appeal for
students in the sixth and eighth grades than for those in the fourth grade.
By the sixth grade, the meaning of authority changes from one implying
purely power to one implying increased ability and personal responsibility.
Having authority was given significantly more emphasis by Lower and
Lower-Middle status children. It may not be power over others but control
over one’s own decisions that is emphasized in the two Upper groups. If
this is the case, authority per se would not be of great importance
to them. On the other hand, the parents of children in the Lower groups are
often dependent upon persons in authority positions for their means of
livelihood. The concept of being "boss" may be considerably more important
in the lives of these children.
Scarcity. Analysis of variance showed that variation in the
weights assigned to scarcity was due to the interaction of the factors of
grade and status level. What appeared to be happening was that the
interpretation placed on the criterion was related to grade; the evaluation
related to status. Younger children tended to give romantic
interpretations; older children saw implications of formal skills and
limited opportunities. A t-test indicated that younger Lower and
Lower-Middle and older Upper and Upper-Middle status students gave this
criterion more weight than the remainder of the students in the sample.
CONCLUSIONS
These findings support the conclusion that patterns of occupational
evaluation found in adult status level sub-cultures are reflected in the
ways children perceive the status structure and function as ascribers of
prestige. Generally, criteria implying material rewards are given more
emphasis by children from Lower or Lower-Middle status backgrounds;
criteria implying psychic rewards, extensive prerequisites, and personal
ability are given greater weight by Upper and Upper-Middle status students.
Developmental factors appear to be operating in the weights assigned to
several of the criteria. In the case of authority and scarcity, these
factors seemed to influence the way in which the child would interpret the
criterion. In the case of fame, they appeared to reflect differentials in
egocentricity.
Some consideration should be given to the limits of generalizability of
these conclusions. The probability values for the various statistics used
refer to a sample of Chicago Public School students. It is to this group
that these conclusions are directly relevant. However, insofar as these
data are consistent with the findings of many researchers in the area of
social stratification, they may be in part independent of local
idiosyncrasy. To this extent, it is hoped that they may be suggestive for
further study of the development of ascription behavior in mass
society.
SUMMARY
The purpose of this study was to determine the relative weights
children assign to various criteria of occupational prestige, and to assess
the effect of their status
background and grade level upon these weights. Through the analysis of
variance of children’s responses to a paired comparisons test, the
following relationships were found:
1. The child’s status level was related to the weights assigned to
income, education, working conditions, fame, and authority. For income,
fame and authority, the weights assigned to the criteria increased as
status level decreased. The direction of the relationship was opposite for
education and working conditions.
2. The child’s grade level was related to the weights assigned to fame
and authority. As grade increased, the importance given fame diminished;
that accorded to authority increased.
3. The interaction of the two variables was related to the weights
assigned to scarcity. Older Upper and Upper-Middle, and younger Lower and
Lower-Middle status students gave this criterion the greatest weight.
These directional patterns were generally congruent with those found in
adult ascription behavior.
1 From , 1956,
19:126–132. By permission.
2 Median family income and median school years completed for population
25 years and older were used in constructing the index. They were combined
by means of conversion to standard
scores.